Click on the cover above
to go to this book
at Amazon.co.uk
|
Don't Shoot the Dog: The New Art of Teaching and
Training
Karen Pryor
Bantam
ISBN 0 553 38039 7
Dont Shoot the Dog has become a classic text in dog training,
and was the first popularisation of operant conditioning for dog trainers and
owners, though others had previously used this technique to train a range of
animals, notably BF Skinner. The idea of operant conditioning had been around
for decades before Dont Shoot the Dog appeared in 1984, but
this book was a key landmark in history of dog training. Karen Pryor is also
usually associated with clicker training, which became popular among many trainers
and owners, especially at competition level, during the 1990s. Why is her book
so popular, and what are its strengths and weaknesses?
The reason this book is so popular probably lies in its offer of a relatively
easy way to train just about any living creature, including children and husbands,
not just dogs. It also fits in with a trend towards using rewards, whereas previously
there was more usage of punishment in training. (Pryor herself distinguishes
between positive reinforcement and rewards, but many trainers use these terms
interchangeably, and I will call her style reward-based training for reasons
later explained.)
This trend towards reward-based training has not only affected dog training
in Western Europe, the US, and Australia, it is part of a wider trend in the
way humans have come to treat children and other creatures that are dependent
on us. Spare the rod and spoil the child was still heard in the
UK in the 1950s, when I grew up, but today there is a wide consensus that children
should not be beaten. Beating children and dogs out of temper was seen as a
bad thing in those days, but some physical chastisement was often
seen as necessary for their proper development. This move away from physical
punishment does not, however, mean that all punishment is a practice of the
past, since there are more ways of punishing others than beating them. Ignoring
children and dogs can be a form of punishment.
Perhaps the move away from the more physical forms of punishment is because
we have become less physical in our everyday lives, for example,
more of us do mental rather than manual labour, and we use computers
to communicate with people in other countries, rather than greeting friends
with handshakes or hugs. It may be that some of the move away from punishment
is illusory, since, though dogs and children are beaten less in the UK these
days, other forms of punishment are often not noticed by those inflicting them.
Strengths of Karen Pryors book include clear explanations of why rewards
can be more effective than punishment, and of the need to reinforce the behaviour
when it is happening. Clickers are useful for this reason, because they give
the dog a signal that what he is doing is right. You can click faster than you
can get out a titbit, and the click doesnt interrupt the dogs activities
in the way that stopping to feed a titbit does.
The emphasis on variable schedules is also useful, since a common mistake of
owners is to reward every desired action with titbits. This can leave owners
feeling helpless when their pockets are empty, and fixed schedules are less
effective than variable schedules. Concepts such as shaping and conditioned
responses (eg using clickers) are also clearly explained. The chapter on Untraining
is especially interesting and helpful, in that several different approaches
are presented, allowing readers to make comparisons and think these issues out
for themselves.
Some people are extremely enthusiastic about this book, and even go so far
as to say that it is the only book that owners need to read. I would, however,
disagree, for a number of reasons.
Firstly, this is not a book for novices. It is not a how to book,
rather it goes into positive reinforcement and operant conditioning in general
in abstract terms with many examples from outside dog training, leaving readers
to work out how to apply it to their own dogs. Novices with a new pup or a rescue
dog are likely to need a little more help.
Secondly, many people may find the reference to dog training in the title confusing,
since Pryor often seems to mention just about any animal other than dogs. In
many ways, her work would have been more useful if she had stuck to relations
between humans and dogs (or other non-humans), and not tried to extrapolate
her ideas to people, presenting operant conditioning as a miracle cure for dealings
between humans. Yes, I agree that sometimes people forget common sense in their
dealings with one another, for example, getting angry with partners who return
home late. (Like dogs, humans tend to return home faster when they have a nice
welcome.) However, our relationships with other humans can go beyond what operant
conditioning has to offer. Cognitive psychology is usually seen as a more effective
way of getting people to change their own and others behaviour, according
to my friends in counselling. Id have liked to see her try to mesh some
of her insights from behavioural psychology with those offered by cognitive
psychology, because, though behaviourism can offer useful insights, it is a
bit limiting. Perhaps this is asking too much of one book, but Pryor does seem
a little too messianic in her zeal to preach operant conditioning as a cure-all,
so I think she lays herself open to such criticisms.
We cant reason with dogs, and here behaviourism has more to offer as
a direct and simple way to get through to them. However there is another gap
that worries me when reading Pryor and other authors of dog books
who stress operant conditioning, and that is their tending to neglect or downplay
the influence of inherited traits, including breed characteristics. Dogs may
also inherit predispositions to be nervous, pushy or whatever, that
are unrelated to breed. And dogs are hardwired to do certain things such as
jump up to greet people, because that is the nature of dogs. A novice might
take Pryors text and think of dogs as a blank sheet, and see all behavioural
problems as a result of inadequate training, or the dog having had some past
experience of being rewarded for a certain (bad) behaviour, or being
frightened, so being bad out of fear. Yet some breeds have inbuilt
tendencies, eg Keeshonds tend to bark a lot. A Keeshond barks because its
in the nature of the beast. Operant conditioning can help to some extent in
controlling the barking, but it wont turn a Keesie into a quiet breed.
Dont Shoot the Dog is not really a good text for learning
about operant conditioning either, I certainly found the explanations of negative
reinforcement confusing, and prefer someone more analytical, like Mary Burch,
who sets out the theory very clearly in How Dogs Learn. Theres
more repetition, more of a polemic, and less of a clear-cut explanation in Dont
Shoot the Dog. Pryor stresses that positive reinforcement is not the same
as giving rewards, but is more effective. However, since the term includes both
signs that a dog is about to be rewarded, and the actual reward, I have no problem
with calling her style reward-based training. Her account would
have been easier to understand if she had simply said that reinforcement involves
stimuli that make behaviour more likely to happen, whereas punishments make
it less likely to happen.
I have found symbols and a schematic presentation helpful in following the
logic behind operant conditioning. The R+ and R- and P+ and P- symbols are especially
useful. All the plus means is that something is added (positive), whereas a
minus means that something is taken away. The R stands for reinforcer, and the
P for punisher. So:
R+ = positive reinforcement = a reward or promise of a reward that makes a
dog more likely to do something (eg titbit for a food-oriented dog),
R- = negative reinforcement, removing something nasty to make it more likely
that a dog will do something (eg slackening a taut leash to help a dog socialize
better),
P+ = positive punishment = punishment that makes a dog less likely to do something
(eg shocking dog to curb chasing), and
P- = negative punishment = taking something nice away to make a dog less likely
to do something (eg owner turning away from jumping up dog, where owners
attention = nice for the dog).
This is what I missed in Karen Pryors explanation of behaviourism. People
learn in different ways and perhaps other readers might have less trouble following
her, but unless I have these concepts set out in a fairly schematic way, I find
them difficult to follow.
There is another criticism that could be made of this book, which is perhaps
a little unfair, but Ill make it anyhow
and that is that its
fairly ethnocentric. The sorts of things we ask of our dogs vary over time and
from place to place. We have more companion dogs, and fewer working dogs, more
pedigrees, and fewer mutts. OK, so most dog books sold in the UK tend to be
ethnocentric, why lambast Karen Pryor? In my case, a) because I happen to be
reviewing her book, and b) because I have lived in rural Spain, and have seen
dogs brought up and kept in very different ways from in the UK. They tend to
be either working dogs, purpose-bred for guarding and hunting, and mostly relatively
isolated from humans, or village mutts, a kind of nondescript smallish type
of dog. These dogs trot round villages minding their own business and bothering
no-one. They tend to avoid people they dont know, and cars. They are also
well-behaved by UK standards. Why? They dont usually get formal training.
Maybe the ones that dont behave well die young - from the kick of a passer
by, or being run over. Dogs that venture into other peoples stockyards
often die of poisoning. So to some extent, bad behaviour is bred out of them.
This link between cultural change and the type of breeds we keep, ties in with
a point made forcefully by the Coppingers in Dogs, which is that
many dog behavioural problems seen in western societies are the result of people
keeping breeds in ways that they are not suited, keeping herding dogs like collies
shut up and bored all day, for example. A dose of operant conditioning for ten
minutes in the evening is not going to be enough for a border collie kept shut
up in a house all day. Its often worth looking at our own culture through
the eyes of another culture to see why we sometimes get things wrong. Im
not proposing keeping small Spanish village dogs and allowing them to run loose.
They would be mowed down fast in urban UK traffic, and tend to have pretty short
lives in rural Spain. I am saying, however, that some of the problems that Karen
Pryor is trying to address have their roots in cultural change, especially fashions
for keeping very specialised working dogs for companionship. This fashion has
also started to extend to rural Spain - by the 1990s there was a fad for keeping
Siberian huskies in the villages in central Spain I know well. Not the best
dog for a region that is hot even by Madrid standards.
In conclusion, Karen Pryors book is a must read for anyone
seriously interested in dog behaviour and training, but it should be supplemented
by other texts. It is not the first book Id recommend for a novice, because
it doesnt have enough in the way of detailed instructions. Ian Dunbars
How to Teach a New Dog Old Tricks is perhaps the best book for a
novice. Dont Shoot the Dog is not analytical enough for someone
who is studying animal behaviour in an academic way. It could also lead people
to have unrealistic expectations of their dog, if they have chosen a breed that
is not suited to the lifestyle they lead. So why bother with it? Firstly because
its an important historical document, secondly because its a good
read, and thirdly because, despite its shortcomings, Dont Shoot
the Dog is very clear on the rationale behind some of the reasons why
certain training techniques tend to be more useful than others.
References:
Mary Burch and Jon Bailey (1999) How Dogs Learn
Raymond and Lorna Coppinger (2001) Dogs
Karen Pryor (1999) Dont Shoot the Dog (revised edition)
A.L. April 2002 |